![]() |
Sepia officinalis is a medicinal plant. (Getty Images/Schafer & Hill/The Picture Bank) |
A recent
test of cephalopod intelligence has highlighted the importance of humans not
underestimating animal intelligence.
Cuttlefish
have been put through a new iteration of the marshmallow test, and the findings
seem to show that their peculiar little brains have more going on than we
previously believed.
Cuttlefish's
ability to learn and adapt may have evolved to give them an advantage in the
cutthroat eat-or-be-eaten aquatic environment in which they live, according to
the researchers.
The
marshmallow test, also known as the Stanford marshmallow experiment, is a
simple one. A marshmallow is put in a room with a boy. They are told that if
they can go 15 minutes without eating the marshmallow, they will be given a
second marshmallow and allowed to eat both.
This
ability to delay gratification reflects cognitive skills such as future
planning, and it was originally conducted to learn more about how human
cognition evolves, specifically when a human is wise enough to delay
gratification if it means a better outcome later.
It can be
adapted for animals because it is so plain. You can't tell an animal that if
they wait, they'll get a better reward, but you can teach them that if they
don't eat the food in front of them right away, better food will come.
Dogs, like
certain primates, may postpone gratification, although infrequently. The marshmallow
test has been passed by corvids as well.
Cuttlefish
have passed a variant of the marshmallow test last year. Scientists discovered
that common cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) will stop consuming crab meat in the
morning if they know that dinner will be something they like - shrimp.
However,
as a team of researchers led by behavioral ecologist Alexandra Schnell of the
University of Cambridge point out in a new paper, it's difficult to tell if
this shift in foraging behavior in response to prey availability was also
governed by self-control in this case.
As a
result, they invented a new test for six common cuttlefish. The cuttlefish were
housed in a special tank with two enclosed chambers with transparent doors that
enabled them to see inside. Snacks were available in the chambers, including a
less-appealing raw king prawn in one and a slightly more appealing live grass
shrimp in the other.
The
cuttlefish had been taught to identify symbols on the doors as well. A circle
suggested that the door would open instantly. The door would open after a time
interval of 10 to 130 seconds if the form was a triangle. A square, which was
only used in the control situation, meant that the door would remain closed
indefinitely.
The prawn
was put behind the open door in the test condition, while the live shrimp was
only accessible after a pause. The shrimp was automatically removed if the
cuttlefish went after the prawn.
Meanwhile,
behind the square-symbol door that wouldn't open in the control group, the
shrimp remained unavailable.
The
researchers discovered that all of the cuttlefish in the test condition wanted
to wait for their desired food (live shrimp), while none of the cuttlefish in
the control group did.
Cuttlefish
in this study were all able to wait for the better reward and tolerated delays
of up to 50-130 seconds, which is similar to what we see in large-brained
vertebrates like chimps, crows, and parrots, according to Schnell.
The
experiment also included a measure of how well the six cuttlefish could
understand. A grey square and a white square were shown to them as visual
clues. If they reached one, the other would be removed from the tank, and they
would be rewarded with a snack if they made the "right" option.
When the
researchers had learned to equate a square with a reward, they reversed the
cues, making the other square the reward cue. Surprisingly, the cuttlefish that
learned to adapt the fastest were also the cuttlefish that could wait longer for
the shrimp reward.
Cuttlefish
seem to be capable of self-control, although the reason for this is unclear.
Delay gratification has been related to factors like tool usage (because it
involves preparation ahead), food caching (for obvious reasons), and social
competence in species like parrots, primates, and corvids (because prosocial
behavior - such as making sure everyone has food - benefits social species).
Cuttlefish
do not use tools or cache food, and they are not very social, as far as we
know. Cuttlefish's ability to postpone gratification may have something to do
with how they forage for food, according to the researchers.
Cuttlefish
spend the majority of their time camouflaging, sitting, and waiting, with
occasional bursts of foraging thrown in for good measure, according to Schnell.
When they
forage, they lose their camouflage, exposing themselves to any predator in the
ocean trying to eat them. We assume that delayed gratification evolved as a
result of this, allowing the cuttlefish to maximize foraging by waiting to pick
higher-quality food.
It's an
intriguing example of how very different lifestyles in very different species
can result in behaviors and cognitive abilities that are remarkably similar.
The team suggests that future studies should focus on determining whether
cuttlefish are capable of preparing for the future.
0 Comments